Conlang
Advertisement


Progress 78%
Churra
cula
Type
Isolating
Alignment
Nominative-Accusative
Head direction
Mixed
Tonal
No
Declensions
Yes
Conjugations
Yes
Genders
No
Nouns decline according to...
Case Number
Definiteness Gender
Verbs conjugate according to...
Voice Mood
Person Number
Tense Aspect



General information[]

Churra is a mostly isolating language with some fusional elements, and a heavy dependence on word order to illustrate many grammatical concepts, including tense. It has an exceptionally large consonant inventory, with many fricatives, but very few vowels. It is spoken in a small coastal region of the planet Ysla, by the Chasrae, an alien species of "crab-taurs" (crustaceans with an upper torso similar to a human) with a tri-sexual reproductive system.

It is peculiar in its large phonemic consonant inventory (68) including some sounds not possible for humans to produce.

Phonology[]

Consonants[]

Bilabial Labio-dental Dental Alveolar Post-alveolar Palatal Velar Uvular Glottal
Ventral* m [~m] n [~n] g [~ŋ]
Plosive b [b] t [t] k [k] q [q]
Fricative f [ɸ] v [v] d [ð] s [s] z [ʒ] j [ɕ~hʲ] x [x], r [ɣ~ʁ] h [h]
Long Fric. ŝ [s:] ĵ [ʃ:~ɕ:] ĥ [x:~h:]
Affricate p [pɸ~pf] c [tʃ]
Approximant w [w] y [j]
Tap or flap l [ɾ~l]
Lateral
  • 'Ventral' sounds are non-pulmonic consonants, formed by the articulation of a stop and the channeling of air through a disparate voice organ that is not available to humans - a secondary, ventral windpipe or 'wind chamber' that is a vestigial remnant of the specie's once-present gills. It gives a rasping, whistling quality to the affected sounds, and can be approximated in human speakers by use of nasals, ejectives, implosives, or clicks at the corresponding point of articulation, which are not available to the species, who have no nasal passage.

Palatalization and Labialization[]

Any consonant can be modified from C to Cʲ or Cʷ (with the exception that /w/ cannot be labialized, and /y/ cannot be palatalized, for obvious reasons; however either can undergo the opposite process). These are phonemic distinctions, effectively tripling Churra's consonant inventory. This is written in the romanized orthography as Ce and Co.

Vowels[]

Front Central Back
Short High i [i] u [u]
Long High î [i:] û [u:]
Short Low a [a]
Long Low â [a:]

Alphabet[]

Churra is written in ideographs and sometimes in a syllabry. Both are under development. The romanized alphabet used is included in the phonemic inventories chart.

Note that in some instances long sounds may simply be written doubled, e.g. ĥ = hh, î = ii. This is not the preferred method since the syllabic consonants can be interspersed with their non-syllabic counterparts, as can be seen in the word xŝsŝ, 'snake,' which with the doubled-character system would need to be transcribed xsssss. Despite what a reader's intuitions may be, the sequence in 'snake' should be composed of two audibly separate syllables, [xs:.ss:] not merely a very long-held s sound [xs:::].

Phonotactics[]

Stress structure[]

Churra, unlike human languages, has a trisyllabic stress pattern with foot-initial stress, leading to the grouping of its syllables into triplet feet; kè-ke-ke-ké-ke-ke. (A tri-trochee?) However, since a large percentage of Churric words are only one or two syllables long (degenerate feet are obviously permitted), the pattern often appears to simply be word-initial stress.

The word-head is final, feet count from left to right, feet are iterative, extrametricality is not permitted, and it is quantity-sensitive; syllables with a long vowel or a vowel followed by a glide are considered heavy.

Syllable structure[]

The basic syllable structure in Churra is CV. However, word-initial vowels and word-final consonants are allowed; this makes it possible to occasionally know word boundaries, where consonant clusters not permitted at the word level occur.

Hiatus is never permitted; if an affix would create hiatus, the first vowel assimilates to the second, resulting in one long vowel; if either vowel is already long, the hiatus will be interrupted by an appropriate glide.

  • when the initial vowel is /a:/ :
    • a:V: → a:V:
      • e.g. â+ûla → âwula
    • a:V → a:G#, GV elsewhere
      • e.g. tâ+u → tâw ; â+iva →yiva
    • aV: → GV:
      • e.g. na+ûdik → noûdik
  • elsewhere
    • VːVː → VGVː | VːV → VGV | VVː → GVː

Most consonant clusters are not permitted, save some exceptions; C+glide (G; phonemicized as labalialized or palatalized Cs), phonemic affricates (/c/ and /p/), and the long fricatives (F:). The long fricatives may function as either a long consonant or a voiceless vowel. In the available syllable structure, any phonemic consonant (except F:) or any short vowel counts as one mora, while long fricatives and vowels count as two. Each syllable may contain a maximum of three mora, although this rule is ignored by word-final consonants. All legal syllables in Churra can therefore be accounted for in the following:

  • (C)V(:)
    • e.g. ku, kû, u, û
  • F:V
    • e.g. ssa
  • (C/V)F:
    • e.g. kss, iss, ss
  • (C)V(:)C#
    • e.g. bat, bât, at, ât

When a structure such as CF:V is attempted, the syllable is broken into two, which results in a short syllabic fricative; *[.kssa] is blocked, but [ks.sa] is permitted.

Regular sound changes[]

  • /hʷ/ = [ɸ]
  • /l/ = [l] /_#
  • /l/ = [ɾ] /elsewhere
  • /p/ = [pɸ] OR [pf] (free var.)

Conditioned sound changes (IPA)[]

  • i > e /j_ , Cʲ_
  • i: > eɪ /j_ , Cʲ_
  • u > o /w_ , Cʷ_
  • u: > oʊ /w_ , Cʷ_
  • u, u: > ʉ, ʉ: /palatal_
  • a, a: > ɑ, ɑ: /_uvular_ , _h_
  • C > C [+voice] /V_V
  • C > C [-voice] /_#
  • ð > θ /F:_
  • h > x /_i
  • h: = x: /_i, _palatal

Grammar[]

Gender Cases Numbers Tenses Persons Moods Voices Aspects
Verb No Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes
Nouns Yes No Yes No No No No No
Adjectives Yes No No No No No No No
Numbers No No No No No No No No
Participles No No No No No No No No
Adverb Yes No No Yes No No No Yes
Pronouns No Yes Yes No Yes No No No
Adpositions No No No No No No No No
Article No Yes No No No No No No
Particle No No No No No No No No


Pronouns & Grammatical Number[]

The pronouns which serve as the verbal arguments in Churra have three distinctions - class, person, and number. The only aspect of Churric nominal declension that the pronouns do not exhibit is gender (animacy).

The pronouns use a two-way class system (subject/object), encode three persons (first, second, and third), and capture the language's five-way number system.

Subject Pronouns
1 2 3
Zero/Neg ix cix xu
Singular lu cu vu
Paucal idi iyi iwi
Plural di yi wa
Mass/Coll tu tiju toa
Object Pronouns
1 2 3
Zero/Neg xeu ju xa
Singular ilu zu
Paucal isoa iya îboa
Plural isi yi bea
Mass/Coll tilu tiyi ta

Despite the fact that all other relevant case-marking elements of Churra make a three-way distinction for Subject, Direct Object, and Indirect Object, the pronouns only have S and O forms. Pronoun order is relied on to convey case: the order is IO-S-DO.

  • taras lu zu
    • kill.TR 1SS 2SO
    • 'I killed you'
  • *taras zu lu
    • kill.TR 2SO 1SS
    • '*I killed to you'
  • *taras lu cu
    • kill.TR 1SS 2SS
    • '*I you killed'
  • *taras cu lu
    • kill.TR 2SS 1SS
    • '*You killed I'
  • taras cu ilu
    • kill.TR 2SS 1SO
    • 'You killed me'
  • *taras ilu cu
    • kill.TR 1SO 2SS
    • '*You killed to me'

Grammatical Number[]

While the meaning behind the number distinctions are more transparent when applied to nouns (see 'the noun,' which also includes the nominal declensions for number), the five-way system allows for a variety of ideas to be communicated.

Zero/Negative[]

The use of Zero/Negative (Z) pronouns in a sentence can convey a similar message as the negative in other languages.

  • ba ix
    • go.IT 1Z.SUBJ
    • 'I/we didn't go'
    • 'None of us went'

Note that in realworld terms, there is no observable difference between 'We didn't go' and 'None of us went' ; neither allows that any of the 'we' group went, and neither precludes that nobody else (none of you/them) went. In general, however, there is also a negative particle which allows for the exact number of non-goers to be stated: ixa.

  • ba ixa lu
    • go.IT NEG 1SG.SUBJ
    • 'I didn't go'

Singular and Plural[]

The singular (SG or just S) and plural (PL or just P) numbers behave nearly identically in Churra as they do in English; they captures the concept of 'one' and 'more than one' (roughly: see 'paucal' below). Therefore, the first person singular subject pronoun lu can be thought of as equivalent to English 'I,' and the plural counterpart is the same as English 'we.'

  • ba lu
    • go.IT 1SG.SUBJ
    • 'I went'
  • ba di
    • go.IT 1PL.SUBJ
    • 'we went'

When used to refer to a mass noun, the singular acts as a singulative.

  • yuf la gumssa. yulssu vu.
    • COP PART.SJ grain. fall.IT 3SG.SJ
    • there is some grain. one grain fell.

Paucal[]

The main difference between plural in English and plural in Churra is that the Churric form conveys, specifically, 'many,' or at least 'more than a few.' This is because it is the paucal (PC)'s job to encode 'a few.'

  • ba idi
    • go.IT 1PC.SUBJ
    • 'a few of us went'

When used in conjunction with mass nouns, the paucal means roughly 'a small percentage':

  • yuf la gumssa. yulssu iwi.
    • COP PART.SJ grain. fall.IT 3PC.SJ
    • there is some grain. some of it fell.

Mass/Collective[]

Mass/collective (C) number is similar to 'noncount' nouns in English; it is used mostly with fluids (e.g. water, air), and large collections of miniscule objects (e.g. grain, rice, sand). However it can also apply to large groups of anything which are then considered a collective (similar to 'a herd of deer' vs 'several deer').

It is worth noting here that the C marker for nouns (see 'the noun') will not always appear on nouns which behave collectively, such as 'water,' but these are nevertheless referred to with collective pronouns.

  • ba tilu
    • go.IT 1C.SUBJ
    • 'all of us went'

Reflexive Pronouns[]

Reflexives
1 2 3
fss fij pa


Since reflexives must be used in conjunction with another pronoun, it is unimportant to know number on a reflexive - these follow their subject.



  • unut lu
    • hit.TR 1SG.SUBJ
    • 'I hit it'
  • unut lu fss
    • hit.TR 1SG.SUBJ 1RFX
    • 'I hit myself'
  • unut di fss
    • hit.TR 1PL.SUBJ 1RFX
    • 'We hit ourselves'
  • unut ix fss
    • hit.TR 1Z.SUBJ 1RFX
    • 'I/we didn't hit myself/ourselves'

The Verb[]

Verbs must show transitivity and tense, and can have modal, aspectual, and further valency information added by means of particles.

Transitivity, Valency & Voicing[]

Each verb has an inherent transitivity feature, and come in series of related verbs to allow for different transitive forms. There are five different types of verb; transitive, intransitive, ditransitive, 'dative,' and 'passive.'

Verb Transitivity
# Args Subject Dir.Obj Ind.Obj Endings
IT 1 Yes No No Vowel
TR 2 Yes Yes No Alveolar, Dental
DT 3 Yes Yes Yes Uvular, Glottal
DA 2 Yes No Yes Labial, L-dental
PA 1 No Yes No Palatal, Velar

The 'endings' are often violated by exceptions. The transitivity feature was once an inflection on the verb, but for most verbs it has become fossilized and non-decompositional.

Inherent transitivity allows third-person-singular pronouns to be optional. If the verb is IT and has no visible subject, that subject is a third person; if the verb is DT with no pronouns attached, then all three persons are third persons.

Example 1: mus_ 'he finished'
form example translation
IT musea he finished
TR mus he finished it
DT musuq he finished it for her
DA musuv he finished for her
PA museax he was finished
Example 2: mus_ lu 'I finished'
form example translation
IT musea lu I finished
TR mus lu I finished it
DT musuq zu lu I finished it for you
DA musuv zu lu I finished for you
PA museax ilu I was finished

Other distinctions and combinations of persons may be made with valency-modifying prefixes (also known as voice markers):

  • sa- indicates passivization of the subject pronoun on TR or DT verbs.
    • sataras ilu
    • kill.TR 1SG.OBJ
    • 'he is killed by me'
  • fe(a)- indicates causation on DT verbs; with fe-, the IO causes the S to act on the DO.
    • featarĥ ilu cu
    • kill.DT 1SG.OBJ 2SG.SUBJ
    • 'he made you kill me'
  • ju- is the counterpart to fe-: the DO causes S to act on IO.
    • jubalir ilu cu
    • speak.DT 1SG.OBJ 2SG.SUBJ
    • 'he made me speak to you'
  • ci- indicates 'dipassive,' or a verb with both a DO and IO.
    • cigufiz ki muzaqoi i mŝa
    • DP.pull.PA the.IO ground the.DO flower
    • 'the flower was pulled from the ground'

Tense[]

Churra has a two-tense system, future and non-future (which will be referred to as 'past' at times for sake of simplicity, despite inaccuracy). There is no morpheme, auxiliary verb, or verb form that indicates tense; instead this is achieved through word order.

In future-tense phrases, the arguments will appear before the verb; in the non-future, the arguments appear after the verb. This is true regardless of the argument form (pronoun vs noun), unless it is an entire embedded clause in a future-tense sentence, in which case the impersonal sub-clause pronouns appears before the verb, and the longer, imbedded clause appears afterwards.

Since the third-person pronouns are optional, many phrases will have no visible arguments or other methods of indicating tense-aspect or temporal information. In this case it is generally assumed that the phrase is in non-future tense, except in certain narrative conditions (e.g. following a future-tense phrase).

  • mouga (run.IT) 'he runs/ran'
  • vu mouga (3SG.SUBJ) 'he will run'
  • mouga lu (1SG.SUBJ) 'I run/ran'
  • yaj lu mouga ss... (3SG.IO) 'I will run to...'

While the non-future corresponds to English past and/or present for the most part, there are some instances where a phrase that would be present in English is future in Churra. This is especially true of phrases with an irrealis mood, or when a verb has consequences that may extend into the future.

Aspect[]

Another important set of distinctions of the Churric verb are its aspects. These always appear directly before the verb (except for voice markers).

Aspects
Future Non-Future
Remote-Perfect/Simple li su
Progressive mi bu
Anterior-Progressive xi ku
Gnomic sa
Stative

na

The first six aspects must, in a way, agree with the phrasal tense. Adding su to a future phrase, for instance, would cause an ungrammatical and infelicitous utterance.

The remote-perfect/simple aspect, frequently shortened to 'remote,' indicates that an event occurred a long time in the past or will occur far in the future. This is therefore one of the few ways to know for sure that a non-future phrase is past, and not present. It has been partially co-opted to merely indicate past tense for this reason.

The anterior-progressive is generally just called 'anterior.'

Gnomic aspect is used to state that an event (or state) is always true, or that a statement is a general (usually well-known) truth, e.g. sa juma tîla 'birds fly.' It is sometimes used as a means of trying to make a sentence seem more truthful and honest.

The stative is used to describe a situation which is ongoing but unchanging.

The semantic difference between the future and non-future of the stative and gnomic is minor at best, which is probably why there is only one form for each.

  • wira lu 'I run/ran'
  • su wira lu 'I ran a long time ago'
  • *li wira lu 'I ran a long time from now'
  • lu li wira 'I will run a long time from now'
  • bu wira lu 'I am/was running'
  • lu mi wira 'I will be running'
  • ku wira lu 'I had been running'
  • lu xi wira 'I will have been running'
  • sa wira lu 'It is true that I run'
  • lu sa wira 'It is true that I will run'
  • na wira lu 'I (have) always run'
  • lu na wira 'I will always run'

Aspects are only allowed to stack if one has future/non-future distinction and the other does not; in this case, the gnomic or stative particle appears before the other. In this method the stative and progressive may be combined to create the pluperfect.

  • sa bu sirif zu lu
  • ST PR.P wait.DA 2SG.OBJ 1SG.SUBJ
  • 'I have been waiting for you'

Mood[]

mood form example translation
imperative mĵ mĵ zu gara look up!
optative leu ara leu ka may the water stay still
hortative tala di tala beu let's go!
debitive uqu cu uqu bala you must/should speak
capability noas noas sa bala lu I can speak

Most modal sentences, especially ones that use irrealis moods, that would take the present in English are in future tense in Churra.

Determiners & Case[]

There are three types of determiners in Churra: articles, demonstratives, and possessives. Articles communicate case and definiteness, while demonstratives encode spacial relation to the speaker or subject. It is possible to use both partitive and/or tool articles and demonstratives together.

Articles[]

definite indefinite partitive Prop. Noun
subject a, at la, lat ma
dir.obj i, ŝ us koa, ku koun
ind.obj ki, kij ĵ keu, kea in
tool tin ti tul

The 'proper noun' articles are only required when the sentence's structure leaves case ambiguous.

  • a mŝa 'the flower (sj)'
  • at alois 'the sun (sj)'
  • mŝa 'a flower (sj)'
  • lat ara 'some still water (sj)'
  • i alois 'the sun (oj)'
  • ki alois 'to/at/etc the sun'
  • tin alois 'with the sun'
  • vujaw utuga icibirilĥk ma Tum
    • laugh.DA monkey PAUC-trick-PSS.IO the.SUBJ Tom
    • 'Tom laughed at the monkey's tricks'

Demonstratives[]

There are 36 demonstratives in Churra, which encode three axes of information: case, proximity, and vertical direction.

Subject Dir.obj Ind.obj
^ = v ^ = v ^ = v
prox ik aq uq is as u ili ala ulu
med nin nan nug nisit nas nus yin yan yun
dist mi ma mou misit mas mu mif mafa muf
surr tid tad tudu tig tag tug tili tal tul

What is immediately apparently is that there is a sound-meaning correspondence between the vowels and the height distinctions (above, equal/eye-level, below). This sound symbolism is present in other parts of the language as well.

The four levels of proximity are proximal, medial, distal, and surrounding, which correspond roughly to English this X, that X, yonder X (cf. Sp. aquell@), and the surrounding X.

  • lu nisit mŝa qapul
    • 1SG.SUBJ DEM flower want.TR
    • 'I want that flower up there'
  • bu wudiwi tal ara lu
    • PR.NF swim.DA DEM still.water 1SG.SUBJ
    • 'I am swimming in still water'

Possessive Case[]

There are two ways to form the possessive in Churra - by using a possessive pronoun (like English my, your, her) or by using the possessive declension. (When using possessive pronouns, but the pronoun and the case marker must be used.) This is the only form of declined case in Churra.

Possessive Endings
Subject Dir.Obj Ind.Obj
-im -ssd -hhk

Unlike in English, these endings are not applied to the possessor (Mary's kitten) but to the possessed. The possessor is left without any article.

  • Mali nanîm 'Mary's kitten (sj)'
  • wazat a kavoa Mali naniŝd
    • chase.TR the.SUBJ dog Mary kitten-PSS.DO
    • 'the dog chased Mary's kitten'
Possessive Pronouns
1 2 3
Sg lum coam vim
Pl dim joam

bim

  • wazat a kavoa vim naniŝd
    • chase.TR the.SUBJ dog 3SG.PSS kitten-PSS.DO
    • 'the dog chased her kitten'

Adpositions[]

These fall under the category of determiners in Churra since they may replace articles and demonstratives in the phrase, standing in place of case or directional marking. They are generally found before the noun they modify.

meaning form example translation
from ŝki a diku ŝki banuku the man from the village
for (purp) cuy dutan cu mougâ stick for walking (walking stick)
for (dur) asa seira di asa boamutatuk we waited for several hours
on/in yal siqĥyum yal ki banuku lu I live in the village
(made) of imŝ misil imŝ qugea door of wood (wooden door)
between ŝkim ŝkim ki irigiqoi us waneut the valley between the mountains

The Noun[]

The noun in Churra has little morphology. However, it does show number, and something akin to animacy.

Number[]

Churra has a five-way number distinction.

number form example translation
zero/neg fe(i)-

feimŝa

no flowers
singular ø mŝa one flower
paucal i- imŝa a few flowers
plural bo(a)- boamŝa several flowers
mass/coll ta- tamŝa all the flowers, a bouquet

The behaviour of these numbers is not entirely predictable to an English speaker (see also 'pronouns and grammatical number,' above).

The 'zero' number behaves almost identically to a negation particle:

  • yuf feimŝa
    • 'COP Z-flower
    • 'there are/is no flower(s)'

Zero nouns act as non-definite nouns, taking no article. One may specify that the X did Y by using another grammatical number, an article, and the negation marker ixa.

  • yuf ixa boamŝa
    • 'COP NEG PL-flower
    • 'there are no flowers'

Singular and plural are nearly-identical to English. The distinction between paucal and plural is best summed up as 'a few' vs 'many/ more than few,' ; the exact number being described depends on context, what the object is, etc. However, the number range for paucal is generally anything between or including two and six.

Many nouns considered collective (AKA 'mass' or 'noncount') in English will be unmarked, and therefore, apparently singular (e.g. ara, 'still water'). However, they still behave collectively (taking collective pronouns and partitive articles).

  • wudiwi keu ara lu. yuf uqura vu.
    • 'swim.DA PART.IO still.water 1SS. COP coldly 3SS.
    • I swam in still water. It was cold.
      • Note that the 3SS pronoun is usually omitted, it is only shown here to illustrate that it is singular.

Nouns that appear with the ta- prefix are usually count nouns that have been made into a group, and now act as one entity.

  • alisaj lu taras la takavoa koa tîla
    • 'see.TR-subc 1SS kill.TR PART.S C-dog PART.DO C-bird
    • I saw (that) a pack of dogs kill(ed) a flock of birds

Animacy/ Word Class[]

There are 19 common Churric noun endings. These are fossilized remnants from a once-productive animacy system, with five major classes of animacy; this was once decompositionable, but it is no more. While the animacy of the noun itself is no longer particularly important grammatically in Churra, there is a correspondence between semi-productive adjective animacy and the nominal form.

Animacy Endings
animacy class form example gloss
person prs1 -ku daku 'person'
prs2 -hax dahax 'child'
prs3 -aj yidâj

'adult'

prs4 -u tilû 'king'
animal an1 -(i)moa dalimoa 'ostritch'
an2 -a ila 'bird'
an3 -voa kavoa 'dog'
an4 -fu radafu 'water beetle'
plant pla1 -tan yutan 'dandelion'
pla2 -sa mŝa 'flower'
pla3 -tea katea 'tall, thin tree'
fluid fl1 -j kij 'fire'
fl2 -soa fŝoa 'turbulent water'
fl3 -ra ixeira 'air'
fl4 -is alois 'sun'
inanimate in1 -ut kamût 'stone'
in2 -qoi naqoi 'ground'
in3 -ag narag 'water (substance)'
in4 -il valil 'place'

Note that adjectives must agree with the animacy of their noun, and not the specific noun class. The specific classes are now completely defunct.

Animacy distinctions might not be completely intuitive to English speakers; for instance both ara 'still water' and fŝoa 'turbulent water' have fluid animacy, while narag 'the substance of water' is inanimate.

Modifiers[]

Churric adjectives are fairly simple, agreeing with their noun's animacy and having no other morphology. The adverbs are slightly more complex, dividing into temporal adverbs (which must agree with tense) and non-temporal adverbs, which behave identically to adjectives.

Adjectives and Non-Temporal adverbs[]

Distribution and morphology[]

These two word classes are only differentiated by their distribution; they are therefore considered the same word class in Churra, and will be referred to as adjectives henceforth for the sake of simplicity.

Churric adjectives follow the noun or verb they modify, and must agree with what they modify, on a six-tier system: verb (adverbs), and the five animacy sets. Therefore, any one English adjective has up to six corresponding words in Churra (and if a form doesn't exist, it can be invented.)

  • sujira 'clear (fl)' ; e.g. jiqoris sujira 'cloudless sky'
  • sujiqoi 'clear (in)' ; e.g. leit sujiqoi 'cloudless day'
  • sujir 'clearly/safely' ; e.g. ixa sujir lu 'I slept soundly'

There are a few exceptions to the N-A order. For one, when a N+A construct is a compound (considered one noun), the order is reversed:

  • kamût hanut 'large rock'
  • hanut kamût 'boulder'

The other exception is colour adjectives, which always precede their noun, and which have only one form, regardless of animacy.

  • zumal kamût 'blue rock'
  • *kamût zumal '*rock blue'

Adjectival Meaning[]

While adjective agreement is mostly just that - agreement - the fact that the classes are differentiated semantically allows different modes of the same adjective to pick out slightly different nuances of meaning, which do not correspond 100% to the English meaning.

fu : 'rotten'
Class Form Possible glosses
Person fuqax ~, infected
Animal fu ~, not edible, infected
Plant futa ~, not edible
Fluid fuj ~, unsafe, poison, spoiled
Inanimate fug toxic, decrepit
Adverbial fuwa similarly to rot or decay

Temporal adverbs[]

Temporal adverbs are inflected as either future or non-future and must also take specific places in the phrase depending on their agreement. In this way they can be considered somewhat similar to aspect or tense markers, since they are time-anchoring agents.

Tense Form Example Translation
future y- yaqalei lu beu 'I will go tomorrow'
non-future te- beu lu teaqalei 'I went yesterday'
(base form) aqalei 'next/previous day'

The root forms of all temporal adverbs must begin with a vowel to allow for this process.

Some temporal adverbs may also function as adjectives (including non-temporal adverbs) but in this case they usually convey spatial information.

  • yimîn 'soon'
  • teimîn 'just now/ recently'
  • imîn 'near'

Vocabulary[]

Swadesh list entries are based on best available semantic counterparts; for example, the animal referred to by kavoa 'dog' is not actually the mammal we know on Earth, but simply a domesticated carnivorous pack animal kept for hunting purposes (which also happens to be an amphibious crustacean).


No. English Churra
1Ilu (s), ilu (o)
2you (singular)cu (s), zu (o)
3hevu (s)
4wedi (s), isi (o)
5you (plural)ji (s), yi (o)
6theywa (s), bea (ob)
7thissee 'demonstratives'
8thatsee 'demonstratives'
9hereiqei
10thereaqalei
11whomuku
12whatmût
13whereuza
14whenuza
15how
16notixa
17allta- (PL marker)
18manybo(a)- (PL marker)
19somei- (PL marker)
20fewi- (PL marker)
21othermoilssm (adj)
22onewun
23twowat
24threeris
25fourit
26fiveanss
27bighagu (prs), hamoa (an), hanea (pla), hanssoa (fl), hanut (in)
28longdaliku (prs), dalivoa (an), dalisa (pla), dalisoa (fl), daleut (in)
29widekuf weilout (in)
30thickcuku (prs), cukimoa (an), cukisa (pla), cukaj (fl), cukag (in)
31heavyumssu (prs), umssa (an), umsstea (pla), umssoa (fl), umssqoi (in)
32smallbix (prs), biwa (an), bssa (pla), biis (fl), beut (in)
33shortvuyaj (prs), vuvoa (an), vuyatan (pla), vuyaj (fl), vuyag (in)
34narrowwij weilout (in)
35thinisuhax (prs), isufu (an), isusa (pla), isura (fl), isoil (in)
36womantadix (prs)
37man (adult male)diku (prs)
38man (human being)
39childdahax (prs)
40wifepilu (prs)
41husbandidilu (prs)
42motherquhax (prs)
43fatherquku (prs)
44animalyumoa (an)
45fishimik (an)
46birdila (an)
47dogkavoa (an)
48louseigafu (an)
49snakexŝsŝ (an)
50wormumoa (an)
51treekatea, katutan, katisa (pl)
52forestsirsstan (pl)
53stickdutan (pl)
54fruitwivi (pl)
55seedqhhtea (pl)
56leafgutea (pl)
57rootsatan (pl)
58barkkurusa (pl)
59flowermssa (pl)
60grassgumisa (pl)
61ropehihil (in)
62skinkuruj (prs), kuruvoa (an)
63meatcirifu (an)
64bloodanax (prs), ana (an)
65bonenuku (prs), nuvoa (an)
66fat
67eggqhhku (prs), qhhfu (an)
68hornhaliku (prs), halea (an)
69tailihax (prs), ifu (an)
70featherilavoa (an)
71hairN/A
72headleijax (prs), leija (an)
73eariliku (prs), ilivoa (an)
74eyemjjiinu (prs), mjjiina (an)
75nosebuku (prs), buboa (an)
76moutharu (prs), arifu (an)
77toothbjjul (in)
78tonguedudu (prs), dudivoi (an)
79fingernailN/A
80footqusstax (prs), qusimoa (an)
81leglalei (prs), ladan (an)
82kneekuvil (in)
83handmikaj (prs), mivoa (an)
84wingadei (an)
85bellytuusu (prs), tuusa (an)
86gutsjilaaku (prs), jilaa (an)
87neckduitax (prs), duita (an)
88backurou (prs), urufu (an)
89breastmagaku (prs), magamoa (an)
90heartum (prs), umoa (an)
91liverloisil (in)
92drinkamoud (fl)
93eatkusa (IT), kusiit (TR), kusay (PA)
94bitedukat (TR), dukag (PA)
95suckmoiji (IT), moijss (TR), moij (PA)
96spitteisdu (IT), teisd (TR), teisduh (DT), teisduv (DA), teisduy (PA)
97vomitwuxu (IT), wuxuz (TR), wuhur (DT), wuxum (DA)
98blowvufi (IT), vuf (DA)
99breathehila (IT), hilis (TR), hilif (DA)
100laughvuja (IT), vujaw (DA)
101seealis (TR)
102hearhizis (TR)
103knowwadira (IT)
104thinkhamija (IT), hamizut (TR)
105smellulhht (TR)
106fearhiqis (TR)
107sleepixu (IT), ixub (DA)
108livesiqhhyu (IT), siqhhyum (DA)
109diealusa (IT), alusaf (IT)
110killtaras (TR), tarhh (DT), tarik (PA)
111fightzimas (TR), zimisiq (DT)
112huntsisa (IT), sisat (TR), sisah (DT), sissk (PA)
113hitunut (TR), unaci (PA)
114cutanut (TR), anutaq (DT), anutay (PA)
115splitwat (TR), watari (DT), wataj (PA)
116stabfssqan (TR), fssq (DT), fssk (PA)
117scratchcazad (TR), cazag (DA)
118digkoina (IT), koin (TR), koinaq (DT), koim (DA)
119swimwudea (IT), wudiwi (DA)
120fly
121walkmouga (IT), mougum (DA)
122comeza (IT), zam (DA)
123lieidirssa (IT)
124sitidssu (IT), idssuf (DA)
125standkixi (IT), kixiv (DA)
126turnmuloa (IT)
127fallyulssu (IT), yulssiv (DA)
128givehhtanjj (DT), hoatam (DA)
129holdaliseas (TR), alisiy (PA)
130squeezesoisoi (TR), soisuyi (PA)
131rubbura (IT), bural (TR), bur (DT), buracss (PA)
132washnara (IT), narît (TR), nar (DT), narax (PA)
133wipetid (TR)
134pullgufid (TR), gufiz (PA)
135pushuqhhat (TR), uqhh (DT), uqhhav (DA), uqjj (PA)
136throwquma (IT), qumit (TR), qumar (DT), qumuq (PA)
137tiewilil (TR), wilih (DT), wiliw (DA), wilix (PA)
138sewahadu (IT), ahad (TR), ahaduh (DT), ahadaw (DA), ahadux (PA)
139countcumea (IT), cumeas (TR), cumeaq (PA)
140saybala (IT), balir (DT), balum (DA)
141singheaza (IT), hizis (TR), hazar (DT), hizum (DA)
142playxisa (IT), xiseu (DA)
143floatyulssi (IT), yulssiv (DA)
144flowsi (IT), siv (DA)
145freezehiifii (IT), hiifss (TR), hiifhh (DT), hiif (DA), hiifjj (PA)
146swellgana (IT)
147sunalois (fl)
148moonN/A
149stariliboil (in)
150waternarag (in), fssoa (fl), ara (fl), amoud (fl)
151raintadis (fl)
152riverlisu (fl)
153lakeakut (in)
154seaiyaas (fl)
155saltqin (in)
156stonekamuut (in)
157sandkusŝ (fl)
158dusthealak (in)
159earthcubil (in)
160cloudwusip (fl)
161fogwusup (fl)
162skyjiquris (fl)
163windixeiri (fl)
164snowkuleaqoi (in)
165icesstileut (in)
166smokewax (fl)
167firejujoa (fl)
168ash
169burnziha (IT), zihas (TR), zihaf (DA), zihaj (PA)
170roadirinag (in)
171mountainrigiqoi (in)
172redkuja
173greenwiisul
174yellowjimit
175whitejir
176blackruta
177nightysst (in)
178dayleit (in)
179yearaluq (in)
180warmhidix (prs), hidi (an), hidea (pla), hidij (fl), hidoit (in), hida (adv)
181colduquj (prs), uqa (an), uqitan (pla), uquj (fl), uqut (in), uqura (adv)
182fullumigu (prs), umimu (an), umina (pla), uminoa (fl), umig (in), umiga (adv)
183newzibu (prs), zivoa (an), zitan (pla), zitara (fl), zeut (in), zibea (adv)
184olduveaj (prs), uvidi (an), uvitea (pla), uvituj (fl), uviit (in), uva (adv)
185goodwu (prs), voa (an), wssa (pla), wura (fl), wumut (in), wumoi (adv)
186badtiku (prs), tifu (an), titan (pla), tiis (fl), tiil (in), tea (adv)
187rottenfuqax (prs), fu (an), futa (pla), fuj (fl), fag (in)
188dirtyatoix (prs), atou (an), atoitan (pla), atouj (fl), atoil (in), atoa (adv)
189straightaamidea (pla), aamik (in), aamea (adv)
190roundtimoa (an), titan (pla), tisoa (fl), teuut (in)
191sharpkasa (pla), kadag (in), keadssa (adv)
192dullmarusa (pla), maruqoi (in), marutss (adv)
193smoothleiluku (prs), leilufu (an), leilutea (pla), leiluj (fl), leiluut (in), leilu (adv)
194wetamar (prs), amara (an), amasa (pla), amara (fl), amaru (in), amat (adv)
195drypisu (prs), pea (an), pisa (pla), pij (fl), peuut (in), peaa (adv)
196correctmacou
197nearimiin
198faraloin
199rightikoi
200leftwin
201atN/A
202inyal
203withtin, ti, tul (case-marking article)
204anduw
205ifN/A
206becauseV-aj ... na yuf, tana
207name


Example text[]

Advertisement